The Importance Of Counting In Early Number Development Education Essay

Published:

This essay has been submitted by a student. This is not an example of the work written by our professional essay writers.

To provide a definition, counting is the action of finding the number of elements of a finite set of objects by continually increasing a counter by a unit for every element in the set, in a certain order. Children make use of counting to show their knowledge of the number names and number system. Archaeological evidence suggests that humans have been using counting for at least 50,000 years, and in ancient cultures counting was used to keep track of early economic data. Learning to count is considered a very important educational and developmental breakthrough in most cultures around the world. Learning to count forms the child's first step into mathematics, and constitutes the most central idea of mathematics. The present essay will attempt to illustrate the importance of counting for the development of number-related skills from an early age (Eves, 1990).

The use of numbers is a skill developed from an early age. In mathematics, there is the term "number sense", a relatively new construct that refers to "a well organized conceptual framework of number information that enables a person to understand numbers and number relationships, and to solve mathematical problems that are not bound by traditional algorithms". Number sense includes some component skills such as number meaning, number relationships, number magnitude, operations involving numbers and referents for numbers and quantities. These skills contribute to general intuitions about numbers and pave the way for more advanced skills (Bobis, 1996).

Studies have shown that this "number sense" begins at a very early age. Even before they are able to count properly, children of around two years of age can indentify one, two or three objects. Theorists as early as Piaget noticed this ability to instantaneously recognize the number of objects in a small group. Piaget called in "subitizing". Later, as the child's mental powers develop, around the age of four, groups of up to four objects can be recognized without counting. Adults have and continue to use the same ability of subitizing, although even they cannot use it beyond a maximum of five objects, unless the objects are arranged in a particular way or practice that aids memorization. Subitizing refers to the mind's ability to form stable mental images of patterns and then associate them with a fixed number. In a familiar arrangement, such as six dots arranged into two rows of three (such as in dice or playing cards) six can be instantly recognized when presented this way (Gelman & Gallistel, 1978).

Yet, with the exception of familiar arrangements such as the examples above, when people are presented with groups numbering more than five objects, they must resort to other mental strategies. Groups can be broken up into sub-groups to facilitate the process. A group of six objects, for example, can be broken up into two sub-groups of three, which are recognized instantly and then unconsciously combined into six, the number of the bigger group. This strategy does not use any actual counting, but a part-part-whole relationship which is assisted by rapid mental addition. Therefore, there is an understanding that a number can be composed of smaller parts, along with the knowledge of how these parts add up. This kind of thinking has already begun by the time children begin school, around six or seven years of age. It should be nurtured and allowed to develop, as it is thinking of this sort that lays the foundation for understanding operations and developing mental calculation strategies (Bobis, 1996).

Skills such as the ability to perceive subgroups, need to be developed alongside counting in order to provide a firm foundation for number sense. Although there is no denying that counting is crucial for the development of numbers, these other skills play an important part as well. Skills and alternative strategies for counting can be developed more effectively by the use of teaching strategies. Children can be shown flashcards with objects in different arrangements (sometimes six in a group of four and a group of two, or sometimes in three pairs) as these different arrangements will tend to prompt different strategies. Furthermore, if the flashcards are shown for only a few seconds, the mind is challenged to act faster and develop strategies other than counting to make the necessary calculations (Way, 1996).

Yet, despite the importance of alternative strategies, a considerable amount of evidence supports the idea that counting is the most important mechanism used by young children in estimating numbers of all sizes, perhaps only with the exception of 1 or 2. Subitizing and grouping, as described above, are used as mediators for the ability to understand small numbers, but it seems that even these skills are developed after children have learned to estimate numbers by counting. Moreover, counting is the basic mechanism used when children learn to add and subtract. At least the initial stages of adding and subtracting, before the child masters the processes, involve counting. For example adding 8 and 3 might be achieved by first counting to 8 and then proceeding to 11 (Gelman & Gallistel, 1978).

A surge of interest in counting was triggered by Gelman and Gallistel's (1978) book, which claimed that preschoolers' learning to count was inexplicable unless they had innate predispositions to learn counting. So, is counting innate or not? Butterworth et al. (2005) believe that the human ability to count is innate and is not reliant on numbers or language to express it. They based their study on the fact that the children of Australian Aborigines were able to count even though their languages do not have words for numbers. It seems like a strict form of linguistic determinism has been developed recently, which states that counting words are necessary for humans to develop concepts of numbers larger than three. In contrast, the team's study of aboriginal children came to the conclusion that the skill of recognizing and representing numerosities and the number of items in a set is innate for humans. If their culture does not provide them with a number vocabulary, this does not stop them from performing numerical tasks without number words.

On the other hand, other cross-cultural studies support the opposite conclusion: counting is not innate. Although it seems to come naturally, counting may be cultural rather than innate. Many hunter-gatherer societies such as the Australian Aborigines or various different peoples in South America have no words in their languages for counting or at best only words for up to the number five. This could be because those societies do not have the culturally supported contexts (i.e. the situations) where the need arises for numbers to be represented by words. To investigate the issue, one study (Hyde et al., in press) examined a population of deaf Nicaraguans who speak their native language and never learned Spanish, nor had they the opportunity to learn a conventional type of sign language. The result is that these people live as part of a numerate culture which utilises exact counting and large numbers, yet because they were never trained in either of these skills, they do not have either of them, nor any ability to use conventional language. Still, despite being part of this culture where representations of numbers larger than three are available and even necessary, these people did not spontaneously develop such a skill. True, they make use of gestures when communicating about numbers, but they fail to produce gestures that refer to sets of larger values than three in a consistent manner. This is in contrast to native speakers of the American Sign Language, who, raised and immersed in a language that uses counting, were just as good as speakers of Spanish and English at counting. Therefore, deafness was not the factor that made the difference.

The overall point, though, is that whether innate or not, there can be little doubt that counting is crucial for early number development. People belonging to those cultures without words for numbers larger than five can subitize up to a point but are handicapped when the need arises to deal with larger quantities (Butterworth et al., 2008). When it comes to children, there is strong evidence that counting, and activities that make use of counting, can be very effective in helping them understand numerical concepts. Even before school age, when the teaching of arithmetic takes place in an organized manner within the school environment, young children are still able to come to grips with counting and benefit from any educational tasks. Even as early as the age of three, some basic principles of counting are already in place. Experiences, especially when under the guidance and supervision of an adult, help children build a pre-formal understanding of mathematics that can be beneficial for their educational development. Counting in particular, can help to strengthen the child's intrinsic tendencies for learning. The highly influential book of Gelman and Gallistel (1978) proposes a set of counting principles, and counting exercises based on these principles contribute greatly to children's pre-formal understanding and progress toward formal understanding.

The principles put forward by Gelman and Gallistel do not refute Piaget's classic, ground-breaking findings on the processes of development, but rather extend them, making use of activities recommended by the famous Swiss psychologist (e.g. classification, seriation, matching, and comparison). Some of these principles are attainable by age three and all of them by age five. The principles are: (1) The one-to-one principle shows that, only one word representing a number is assigned to each item. (2) The stable order principle shows that, words representing numbers are always used in the same order. (3) The cardinal principle shows that the last word representing a number which is used, is also the number of items within the set being counted. (4) The order irrelevance principle shows that order is irrelevant when counting. The important thing is that all the items are counted, not the order in which this takes place. Finally, (5) the abstraction principle shows that all the other four principles apply when counting any unique set of items, as well as they do to any other unique set (Aubrey, 1993).

Researchers as early as Beckmann (1924) analyzed the way in which children arrived at an accurate estimate of the number of items, in order to establish the importance of counting. Depending on their behaviour during a counting task and their explanation of how they reached the answer, Bechmann divided the children into "counters" or "subitizers". In general, it was found that the younger the child, the greater the tendency to count for all numbers, while the larger the number, the greater the tendency for all children to count. These results together showed that children estimate a number by counting before they can subitize the same number. Similar effects were observed by Brownwell (1928) and McLaughlin (1935). By asking children to identify the number of elements in arrays of 3 to 10 objects, Brownwell noticed that young children almost always counted and rarely took advantage of the patterns in the display. McLaughlin similarly observed that 3- to 6-year-olds typically counted in order to determine the number of objects in an array, even when the number of objects was small. As the number of items a child could count increased, so did the ability to estimate numbers.

Gelman (1972) notes that when the performance of children in experiments where they have counted is compared with that in experiments where they did not count, the resulting discrepancy adds support to the hypothesis that young children initially estimate by counting. Buckingham and MacLatchy's (1930) study on estimation showed children a random throw of objects, and the subjects were not prevented from counting. In contrast, in Douglas' (1925) study where three similar number tasks were used, children were discouraged from counting. If the groups of 6-year-olds in these and other studies that accordingly encourage or discourage counting are compared, a large discrepancy can be observed. In the first case, the percentage of children who accurately estimated non-linear arrays of around 10 items on at least one trial varied from 54% to 70%, while in the latter case only 8% of the children successfully estimated the numerosity of 10-element arrays. Although the studies differed in a variety of ways, the similarity of the tasks, the selection of the same age group and the use or absence of counting suggest that at least some part of this impressively large discrepancy in successful estimation scores can be attributed to the presence of absence of counting.

Overall, the role of counting on early number development is not entirely clear and there are many different, often conflicting, opinions on how these processes occur. The most striking example is whether counting is innate or not, with some researchers claiming that humans are born with the ability to see the world numerically in the same way that they are born with the ability to see the world in colour, and others insisting that it is a cultural, not an innate ability which will not develop outside of a cultural setting that reinforces it. Different opinions also exist in the matter of the importance of counting and the importance of other skills such as subitizing. Subitizing and other similar skills that assist in estimations are crucial, but they only seem to be so when used together with counting. Counting develops first and produces much better results in estimates and numerical tasks in general. It is the first mechanism used in estimation, the most effective one, and also equally crucial when developing other, more complicated numerical skills such as adding and subtracting. It truly seems to be the basis of early number development.

Writing Services

Essay Writing
Service

Find out how the very best essay writing service can help you accomplish more and achieve higher marks today.

Assignment Writing Service

From complicated assignments to tricky tasks, our experts can tackle virtually any question thrown at them.

Dissertation Writing Service

A dissertation (also known as a thesis or research project) is probably the most important piece of work for any student! From full dissertations to individual chapters, we’re on hand to support you.

Coursework Writing Service

Our expert qualified writers can help you get your coursework right first time, every time.

Dissertation Proposal Service

The first step to completing a dissertation is to create a proposal that talks about what you wish to do. Our experts can design suitable methodologies - perfect to help you get started with a dissertation.

Report Writing
Service

Reports for any audience. Perfectly structured, professionally written, and tailored to suit your exact requirements.

Essay Skeleton Answer Service

If you’re just looking for some help to get started on an essay, our outline service provides you with a perfect essay plan.

Marking & Proofreading Service

Not sure if your work is hitting the mark? Struggling to get feedback from your lecturer? Our premium marking service was created just for you - get the feedback you deserve now.

Exam Revision
Service

Exams can be one of the most stressful experiences you’ll ever have! Revision is key, and we’re here to help. With custom created revision notes and exam answers, you’ll never feel underprepared again.